In order to understand an English sentence, the START system needs to have access to morphological, syntactic, and semantic information about the words in the sentence. All the words that the system is aware of, along with information about their part of speech, inflection, gender, number, etc. are stored in the Lexicon. Virtually every branch of START uses the Lexicon to accomplish its task. In this section we discuss the way in which the Lexicon extends the system's ability to deal with semantic-syntactic interdependencies. We show that the Lexicon provides a place where a verb's membership in a semantic class can be registered, allowing more general S-rules to be stated.
Note that formulating a special purpose S-rule which applies only to the verb surprise does not seem to be the best solution to the problem. Surprise is only one of many verbs which exhibit the so-called property-factoring alternation. This alternation holds of a large class consisting of over one hundred verbs, among them
(8)
amuse, anger, annoy, disappoint, embarrass,
frighten, impress, please, scare, stun, ...
These verbs also share a certain semantic property: they all
denote emotional reactions. For this reason we identify a class
of emotional-reaction verbs and say that the property of the
verb surprise responsible for the alternation shown in (3) and (5) holds for all verbs that
comprise the emotional-reaction class.
Once we have tied the ability to participate in the property-factoring
alternation to a particular class of verbs, we no longer need to
indicate this property in the lexical entry of each verb in the class
or write verb-specific S-rules, such as the Surprise S-rule.
Rather, we can associate the alternation with the emotional-reaction
class and then simply indicate in the lexical entry of a verb whether
it belongs to this class. That is, we augment a verb's lexical entry
with an indication of its semantic class membership. For instance, we
would register in the entry for surprise that it is a member of
the emotional-reaction class. Now instead of writing a number of
verb-specific S-rules, we can write a single general S-rule which
triggers on any verb from the emotional-reaction class:
(9)
Property-factoring S-rule
If <<subject verb object1> with object2>
Then <object2 verb object1>
Provided verb
The revised S-rule contains a Provided clause which
specifies the class of verbs to which the rule applies,
ensuring that it applies to the emotional-reaction verbs.
Provided clauses may impose restrictions on
any of the S-rule variables.
S-rules are based on the observation that semantics intrudes into syntax
in an interesting way, controlling the allowable lexical alternations.
For example, consider the verbs "surprise" and "present;" the
following pairs of sentences show allowable alternative lexical
renditions for each of these verbs:
(10)
Bill surprised Hillary with his answer.
Bill's answer surprised Hillary.
Hillary presented Bill with a gift.
Hillary presented a gift to Bill.
However, as the following non-sentences indicate, neither of these verbs
can support the same lexical alternatives as the other:
(11)
* Hillary's gift presented Bill.
* Bill surprised his answer to Hillary.
This is because these verbs are in different semantic categories. Any
verb having to do with emotional reactions can support the same
lexical alternations as `"urprise". Similarly any verb having to do
with change of ownership can support the same alternations as
"present." START has exploited this phenomenon to achieve a compact
representation of its lexicon.
However, it is just as important to realize that such lexical
information provides significant clues about the similarities and
distinctions which must be made by HPKB's core ontologies and domain
theories. We think that this is an additional value that our project
with its focus on Natural Language can bring to the HPKB program.
emotional-reaction class
Next: Natural Language Annotations
Up: How START Works
Previous: Introducing S-rules
Boris Katz
Thu Apr 17 17:51:51 EDT 1997